Hasil (
Bahasa Indonesia) 1:
[Salinan]Disalin!
BAB 1Psikologi memoriAlan D. BaddeleyJurusan psikologi, University of York, UKDalam bab ini saya akan mencoba untuk memberikan gambaran singkat mengenai konsep-konsep dan teknik yangyang paling banyak digunakan dalam psikologi memori. Meskipun mungkin tidak muncul untuk menjadikasus dari sampling literatur, sebenarnya ada banyak perjanjian mengenai apaPsikologi memori, banyak dikembangkan melalui interaksi yang merupakanStudi normal memori di laboratorium dan yang kerusakan otak yang rusak pasien.Account yang agak lebih rinci dapat ditemukan di Parkin & Leng (1993) dan Baddeley(1999), sementara overviewis lebih luas yang diberikan oleh Baddeley (1997), dan dalam berbagaiBab-bab yang terdiri dari buku pegangan memori (Tulving & Craik, 2000).FRAKSINASI MEMORIKonsep manusia memori sebagai Fakultas kesatuan yang mulai serius terkikis ditahun 1960-an dengan proposal tersebut bahwa jangka panjang memori (LTM) dan memori jangka pendek (STM)mewakili sistem terpisah. Di antara bukti terkuat bagi pemisahan ini adalahkontras antara dua jenis pasien neuropsychological. Pasien dengan klasik amnesicSindrom, biasanya terkait dengan kerusakan lobus temporal dan hippocampi, munculmemiliki masalah yang cukup umum dalam belajar dan mengingat materi baru, Apakah verbalatau visual (Milner, 1966). Mereka melakukannya, namun, tampaknya memiliki memori jangka pendek yang normal(STM), as measured for example by digit span, the capacity to hear and immediately repeatback a unfamiliar sequence of numbers. Shallice &Warrington (1970) identified an exactlyopposite pattern of deficit in patients with damage to the perisylvian region of the lefthemisphere. Such patients had a digit span limited to one or two, but apparently normalLTM. By the late 1960s, the evidence seemed to be pointing clearly to a two-componentmemory system. Figure 1.1 shows the representation of such a system from an influentialmodel of the time, that of Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968). Information is assumed to flow fromthe environment through a series of very brief sensory memories, that are perhaps bestregarded as part of the perceptual system, into a limited capacity short-term store. Theyproposed that the longer an item resides in this store, the greater the probability of its transferto LTM. Amnesic patients were assumed to have a deficit in the LTM system, and STMpatients in the short-term store.The Essential Handbook of Memory Disorders for Clinicians. Edited by A.D. Baddeley, M.D. Kopelman and B.A. Wilson.C 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. ISBN 0-470-09141-X.2 A.D. BADDELEYEnvironmentalinputSensory registersVisualAuditoryShort-term store(STS)Temporaryworking memoryLong-term store(LTS)Permanentmemory storeControl processes:RehearsalCodingDecisionRetrieval strategiesResponse outputHapticFigure 1.1 The model of human memory proposed by Atkinson & Shiffrin. Reproduced fromAtkinson & Shiffrin (1968)By the early 1970s, itwas clear that the model had encountered at least two problems. Thefirst of these concerned the learning assumption. Evidence suggested that merely holdingan item in STM did not guarantee learning. Much more important was the processing thatthe item underwent. This is emphasized in the levels-of-processing framework proposed byCraik&Lockhart (1972). They suggested that probability of subsequent recall or recognitionwas a direct function of the depth to which an item was processed. Hence, if the subjectmerely noted the visual characteristics of a word, for example whether it was in upperor lower case, little learning would follow. Slightly more would be remembered if theword were also processed acoustically by deciding, for example, whether it rhymed witha specified target word. By far the best recall, however, followed semantic processing, inwhich the subject made a judgement about the meaning of the word, or perhaps related itto a specified sentence, or to his/her own experience.THE PSYCHOLOGY OF MEMORY 3This levels of processing effect has been replicated many times, and although the specificinterpretation proposed is not universally accepted, there is no doubt that a word or experiencethat is processed in a deep way that elaborates the experience and links it with priorknowledge, is likely to be far better retained than one that receives only cursory analysis.The effect also occurs in the case of patients with memory deficits, making it a potentiallyuseful discovery for those interested in memory rehabilitation, although it is important toremember that cognitive impairment may hinder the processes necessary for such elaboration.Indeed, it was at one point suggested that failure to elaborate might be at the root ofthe classic amnesic syndrome, although further investigation showed this was not the case(see Baddeley, 1997, for further discussion).A second problem for the Atkinson & Shiffrin model was presented by the data on STMpatients that had initially appeared to support it. Although such patients argued strongly fora dissociation between LTM and STM, the Atkinson & Shiffrin model assumed that STMwas necessary, indeed crucial, for long-term learning, and indeed for many other cognitiveactivities. In fact, STM patients appeared to have normal LTM, and with one or two minorexceptions, such as working out change while shopping, had very few everday cognitiveproblems.This issue was tackled by Baddeley & Hitch (1974), who were explicitly concerned withthe relationship between STM and LTM. A series of experiments attempted to block STMin normal subjects by requiring them to recite digit sequences while performing other tasks,such as learning, reasoning or comprehending, that were assumed to depend crucially uponSTM. Decrement occurred, with the impairment increasing with the length of the digitsequence that was being retained, suggesting that STM and LTMdid interact. However, theeffect was far from dramatic, again calling into question the standard model. Baddeley &Hitch proposed that the concept of a simple unitary STM be replaced by a more complexsystem which they termed “working memory”, so as to emphasize its functional importancein cognitive processing. The model they proposed is shown in Figure 1.2.Working memory is assumed to comprise an attentional controller, the central executive,assisted by two subsidiary systems, the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad.The phonological (or articulatory) loop is assumed to comprise a store that holds memorytraces for a couple of seconds, combined with a subvocal rehearsal process. This is capableof maintaining the items in memory using subvocal speech, which can also be used toconvert nameable but visually presented stimuli, such as letters orwords, into a phonologicalcode. STM patients were assumed to have a deficit in this system, whereas the remainderof working memory was assumed to be spared (Vallar & Baddeley, 1984). Subsequentresearch, based on STM patients, normal children and adults, and children with specificlanguage impairment, suggest that the phonological loop system may have evolved for thepurpose of language acquisition (Baddeley et al., 1998). A more detailed account of thissystem and its breakdown is given by Vallar & Papagno (2002).CentralexecutivePhonologicalloopVisuospatialsketch-padFigure 1.2 The Baddeley & Hitch model of working memory. Reproduced from Baddeley &Hitch (1974)4 A.D. BADDELEYThe visuospatial sketchpad (or scratchpad) is assumed to allowthe temporary storage andmanipulation of visual and spatial information. Its function can be disrupted by concurrentvisuospatial activity and, as in the case of the phonological loop, our understanding hasbeen advanced by the study of neuropsychological patients. More specifically, there appearto be separate visual and spatial components, which may be differentially disrupted. A moredetailed account of this system and the relevant neuropsychological evidence is given byDella Sala & Logie (2002).The third component of the model, the central executive,was assumed to provide an attentionalcontrol system, both for the subsystems of working memory and for other activities.Baddeley (1986) suggested that a good account of it might be provided by the supervisoryattentional system (SAS) proposed by Norman & Shallice (1986) to account for the attentionalcontrol of action. They assume that much activity is controlled by well-learned habitsand schemata, guided by environmental cues. Novel actions that were needed to respondto unexpected situations, however, depended upon the intervention of the limited-capacitySAS. This was assumed to be capable of overriding habits so as to allow novel actions inresponse to new challenges. Slips of action, such as driving to the office rather than the supermarketon a Saturday morning, were attributed to the failure of the SAS to override suchhabits. The problems in action control shown by patients with frontal lobe damage werealso attributed to failure of the SAS; hence, perseverative activity might reflect the failureof the SAS to break away from the domination of action by environmental cues (Shallice,1988).Both Shallice himself and others have extended their account to include a range of potentiallyseparable executive processes, hence providing an account of the range of differingdeficits that may occur in patients with frontal lobe damage (Baddeley, 1996; Duncan, 1996;Shallice & Burgess, 1996). Given the far from straightforward mapping of anatomical locationonto cognitive function, Baddeley & Wilson (1988) suggested that the term “frontallobe syndrome” be replaced by the more functional term, “dysexecutive syndrome”. For arecent review of this area, see Roberts et al. (1998) and Stuss & Knight (2002).The implications of frontal lobe function and executive deficit for the functioning ofmemory are substantial, since the executive
Sedang diterjemahkan, harap tunggu..
