Hasil (
Bahasa Indonesia) 1:
[Salinan]Disalin!
structural Effects in Education 117difficulties that would subsequently emerge: when a variety of school properties are treatedsimultaneously with aggregate status under the rubric of climate, it is impossible to tell whetherschool climate or other conditions correlated with it influence the outcome.In contrast, Rogoff's [Rams0y] (1961) analysis of the same data was an attempt to identifythe "social process," sequence of events, and "social mechanisms" (p. 241) by whichamount of education is directly related to adult social status. She considered three explanations:family influences on academic motivation, the positive scholastic impact of schools onable students whatever their class origins, and community compositional influences both insupport of schools as cultural institutions and manifest in school normative climate (pp. 242-243). With respect to structural effects, she noted the importance of the classroom, through themechanism of its rewards and punishments, as an influence on ability and motivations and asresulting in differing levels of achievement; that is, interaction effects between talent and theschool's system of rewards. She also considered the contextual effects of town, suburban, andcity school location, presumably operating through normative climates. However, why climatesshould vary by type of community is not clear. Rogoff was clearly interested in theproblem of mechanisms and saw them residing in the "educational experience" (p. 250) thatschools provide. However, in the absence of direct evidence about the classroom and its "reward-punishment system," and of the normative impact of communities and schools, mechanismsremained conjectural.Boyle (1966a), reviewing earlier studies of socioeconomic composition effects, askedwhy there should be contextual effects and by what mechanisms they occurred: peer groupculture, the pedagogical characteristics of schools, values and attitudes, or scholastic abilities(p. 628). He devised two explanatory arguments: first, schools differ in their success at developingknowledge and skills; second, they influence attitudes, values, and motivation to attendcollege. Concerning the first, Boyle drew upon Rogoff's (1961) idea that the structure ofnational educational systems (p. 631) should be taken into account (e.g., the decentralizedcharacter of the American system, allowing variation in educational quality school-by-school,in contrast to the more provincially centralized Canadian system). Second, he alluded to thenormative pressure generated among students related to the social-class composition of schools.Limitations of his data prevented B oyle from employing satisfactory measures of instructionaladequacy. He ended up interpreting his evidence as Wilson would have ("students in generallymiddle-class schools who lack . . . [a strong academic background] appear to be carried alongby the majority" [p. 634]; note the similarity to Berelson and associates' concept of the "breakageeffect" [1954, pp. 98-101]). In so doing he presented a structural effects argument thatspanned several levels of educational systems including the knowledge-imparting curricularfunction of schools in addition to normative climate transmitted through interaction.By the late 1960s, the drift of investigations revealed the widespread currency of structuraleffects arguments, but Sewell and Armer (1966a) expressed doubt about them. Theirposition resembled Wilson's: neighborhood contexts represent subcommunities reflected inthe composition of student populations within which normative climates form that influencethe aspirations of all youth, irrespective of individual social status and ability (pp. 161-162).Sewell and Armer maintained, however, that neighborhood context made little contribution toexplaining college plans independent of "traditional variables" (p. 167), an assessment affirmedby Brown and House (1967) and by Hauser (1970, 1971). Their conclusion also tooka poke at recent claims about neighborhood effects, which "may be traced to popular assessmentsof American education by various educational authorities" (p. 160), a snide reference toConant (1961) and his book. Slums and Suburbs.Proponents of contextual analysis (Boyle, 1966a; Michael, 1966; Turner, 1966) leapt to118 Robert Dreebentheir own defense; Sewell and Armer (1966b) rejoined. Most of this debate addressed technicalitiesof definition, of method, of design, of inference, and of causal order. Little of it addressedthe substantive meaning of context and what happens in neighborhoods and schools,which, according to Sewell and Armer, "need to be measured directly and appropriately ratherthan inferred from the social-class composition of the school or the neighborhood" (p. 711;my italics). Michael (1966), taking umbrage at Sewell and Armer's (1966b) dig at educationalauthorities, thought that sociological paternity should prevail because Durkheim's Suicidewas published sufficiently long ago "to elevate contextual analysis from the status of fad totradition" (1966, p. 706). Aside from the parties to this debate airing technical matters, Sewelland Armer (1966b) raised an important point about structural effects arguments (but withoutsupplying substantive remedies): the need for more direct measurement of what context meansand less reliance on conjectural inference from compositional measures.The period from the late 1960s through the 1980s witnessed further developments in thearea of structural effects, some extending and elaborating it, others questioning its conceptualand methodological soundness. Earlier, Robinson (1950) aimed a salvo at its underpinnings.He doubted "whether ecological correlations can validly be used as substitutes for individualcorrelations" (p. 357), except under unlikely conditions, because a correlation between twocharacteristics of individuals in a population will not necessarily be the same when calculatedin subgroups. Hauser (1970) extended this line of criticism with a broader methodologicalcritique in "Context and Consex: A Cautionary Tale" and in a more complete and detailedstatement in Socioeconomic Background and Educational Performance (1971). The logic ofhis argument was similar, the intent different. He was more concerned with how to establishstructural conditions as causes of individual conduct than with whether ecological correlationwill serve as a defensible expedient when individual-level data are unavailable.The contextual fallacy occurs when residual differences among a set of social groups, which remainafter the effects of one or more individual attributes have been partialed out, are interpreted in termsof social or psychological mechanisms correlated with group levels of one of the individual attributes.(1970, p. 659; my italics)Criticizing Blau, Hauser (1970) noted that equating group differences with "the social"and individual differences with "the psychological" represented "a misunderstanding of statisticalaggregation and of social process" (p. 13). He was mainly concerned with properlymodeling social process, jointly implicating both individual and structural considerations,though he did not construct a substantive argument about school organization and schooling.(Blau's analysis, however, did not really depend on a sharp distinction between the "social"and the "psychological.") Hauser questioned the defensibility of contextual arguments: theycan be arbitrary, because selecting one contextual variable does not rule out the appropriatenessof others or of individual explanations. They fail to identify internal mechanisms becausethey assign school-level properties to all students and do not distinguish selection based onthe dependent variable from a contextual effect (Hauser, 1971, p. 32, 1974). A key issue waswhether mechanisms can be identified, a point where Hauser joined Sewell and Armer.Campbell and Alexander (1965) also found difficulty with structural effects argumentsover the issue of mechanisms. They drew on earlier research that emphasized the significanceof personal interaction with school peers. " . . . [AJnalyses of 'structural effects,"' they maintained,must move "from the characteristics of the total system to the situation faced by theindividual due to the effects of these characteristics and then from the social situation confrontingthe individual to his responses to it" (pp. 284-285). Context represented opportunitiesfor interaction with friends planning to attend college. The larger the pool of high socioeconomicstatus peers in the school, the greater the chance of finding such friends (an argument
Sedang diterjemahkan, harap tunggu..
