Hasil (
Bahasa Indonesia) 1:
[Salinan]Disalin!
Even J. G. v. Herder, the leading opponent of Enlightenmentuniformitarianism, was no believer in radical alterity,but only in the importance of culture and historicaltraditions. His Hebrews, Greeks, Africans, Laplanders,and so on had the same sorts of motivations and emotionsas all the rest of humanity, but he believed in the need tounderstand the distinctive ways in which their social andnatural environments had formed their particular associations,understanding, and reactions. Beyond the differencesthat he wrote so much about, he believed ferventlyin a common humanity (Humanitat), and he struggled tocombine and balance this with his view of cultural difference.Barnard writes (1965:98), "Nevertheless the viewcan be upheld and this is what Herder undoubtedly hadat heart that 'relativism' does not necessarily precludethe sharing of certain common attitudes or 'propensities,'sufficient for some degree of understanding between different peoples and generations,regarding the standards towhich they ought to aspire as human beings" (cf. the Boasquotation, note 8).Lewis Henry Morgan was a descendant of the ScottishEnlightenment uniformitarians, and E. B. Tylor believedthat "the uniformity which so largely pervades civilizationmay be ascribed, in great measure, to the uniform actionof uniform causes" (1871, I:1). Tylor's approach tothe science of culture was influenced by the scientisticuniformitarianism of Auguste Comte's positivism. Positivismis, of course, now considered to be one more ofthose intellectual structures of domination, but at least itmight be absolved from the sin of imposing radical alterity.Franz Boas led American anthropology away from Enlightenmentand Victorian evolutionism but not towardradical alterity. In The Mind of Primitive Man (191 1), heargued that all human minds work very similarly, differingonly in the historically derived cultural materials withwhich they have to work. in the same sorts of irrationalities,fears, loyalties, and bondage to received ideas that onemight find in the primitive mind were to be found, for example,in the minds of American undergraduates andeven in his own mind now and then. In fact he was quitecritical of "the judgment of the intellectuals, which ismuch more certain to be warped by unconscious control oftraditional ideas"( [1918]1945:139). He felt strongly thatcross-cultural translation and understanding was possible,indeed necessary.Boas believed that anthropologists and psychologistsshould study others (small o), and not only Westemers, precisely because he believed that our minds work similarly.But, as he wrote in a review of a book by AdolphBastiano n cross-cultural psychology,our reasoning is not an absolutely logical one but. . . it is influenced by the reasoning of our predecessors and by our historical environment: therefore our conclusion and theories, particularly when referring to our own mind,which itself isaffected by the same influences to which our reasoning is subject,cannot be but fallacious. In order to give such conclusions asound basis it is absolutely necessary to study the humanmind in its various historical, and speaking moregenerally, ethnic environments. By applying this method, theobject to be studied is freed from the influences that governthe mind of the student[.1 887:284e, mphasias dded]In other words, The Other R Us, and for that reason, instudying others we are not studying some exotic life formsbut ourselves in different settings. If we fail to considerpeoples and traditions other than our own we distort ourunderstanding of what it means to be human. It is preciselyto guard against the very natural human assumption thatwhat "we" do, based on "our" culture and history, is thenatural, the only, the universal way, that we try to includeas much of the range of human behavior in our accountsand theories as possible.It is odd that this simple idea is no longer widely understood,having been replaced by the notion that we study"Others" in order to feel superior to them and dominatethem. It is remarkable that the lifelong vocation of Boas,Benedict, Herskovits, Mead, and so many others, to combatracism and ethnocentrism, once recognized as a centralelement in American anthropology's legacy, is noweither ignored or made to seem ignoble.My own introduction to anthropology in 1953 was in acourse by Robert A. Manners. Bob Manners and his cohort(including Eric Wolf, Sidney Mintz, Morton Fried,Elman Service, and Marvin Harris) were students of JulianSteward and were part of that post-Boasian generationthat wanted to return to a particular form of Enlightenmentsocial science concerned with parallel sociocultural developments,with cause and effect relations, and with thesearch for laws in culture. Forty-five years ago, when Istudied with Bob Manners, I sensed considerable resistance,discomfort, perhaps even hostility to the idea thatthere are (or can be, or should be) deep-seated and longlastingcultural differences between peoples. His was anattitude as different from an insistence on radical alterityas one can imagine. But it will not do to claim that thissmall band was exceptional, that they were the only oneswho found Melville Herskovits's or Ruth Benedict's versionsof cultural relativism questionable. They were important exemplars and propagators of a well-established and strongly represented point of view in the anthropologyof the 1950s. In many ways it was a dominant view atthat time, but, to repeat, anthropology has always beenpluralistic.
Sedang diterjemahkan, harap tunggu..
